Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/29487
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorRichardson, G. P.-
dc.date.accessioned2019-05-13T13:53:09Z-
dc.date.available2019-05-13T13:53:09Z-
dc.date.issued1991-
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/29487-
dc.description.abstractThe thesis examines the role of verbal labels in category learning by adults. In an investigation of the effects of category learning and exemplar labelling on quantitative judgements about exemplars, it was found that only labels provided at the time of testing biassed subjects’ judgements, although this effect did rely on the labels having been previously learned Adults' default assumptions concerning the extension of novel names among newly learned categories were examined: subjects used an assumption of mutual exclusivity between names (as predicted by Markman, 1989), but did not adhere to the principle of linguistic contrast (Clark, 1987). In a series of experiments where exemplars were labelled with verbal (non-word) labels or complex visual patterns, category learning was superior with the verbal labels. This superiority spanned categories consisting of arbitrary collections of familiar or unfamiliar objects, and prototype-based polygon categories. Arbitrary collection learning was better when subjects reported inventing names for the non-verbal labels. When the verbal and non-verbal labels were compared on a speeded discrimination task, few errors were made but decision times were reliably shorter (c. 0.1 second) with the verbal labels. With other non-verbal labels which were faster to discriminate than the verbal labels, arbitrary collection learning was at a level intermediate between learning with the verbal arid original non-verbal labels. The role of category names as feedback was investigated in a prototype-based category learning task. Learning was no better with named exemplars or right-wrong feedback than when unaided, although learning with named exemplars plus right-wrong feedback was better than with right-wrong feedback only. No interaction between task difficulty and feedback conditions was found (cf. Homa and Cultice, 1984). Thus in these experiments verbal labels produced better category learning than non-verbal labels, even for schema-based categories which were learned equally well unaided. Four possible functions of category names in category learning are suggested as a framework for future investigations.en_GB
dc.language.isoenen_GB
dc.publisherUniversity of Stirlingen_GB
dc.subject.lcshCognitive learning theoryen_GB
dc.subject.lcshCategorization (Psychology)en_GB
dc.subject.lcshRecognition (Psychology)en_GB
dc.titleCategory names and category learningen_GB
dc.typeThesis or Dissertationen_GB
dc.type.qualificationlevelDoctoralen_GB
dc.type.qualificationnameDoctor of Philosophyen_GB
Appears in Collections:Psychology eTheses

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
Richardson.pdf7.47 MBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is protected by original copyright



Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.