Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/27509
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorTajadura-Jiménez, Anaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorVakali, Mariaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorFairhurst, Merle Ten_UK
dc.contributor.authorMandrigin, Alisaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorBianchi-Berthouze, Nadiaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorDeroy, Opheliaen_UK
dc.date.accessioned2018-07-19T15:01:21Z-
dc.date.available2018-07-19T15:01:21Z-
dc.date.issued2017-12en_UK
dc.identifier.other5748en_UK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/27509-
dc.description.abstractMental body-representations are highly plastic and can be modified after brief exposure to unexpected sensory feedback. While the role of vision, touch and proprioception in shaping body-representations has been highlighted by many studies, the auditory influences on mental body-representations remain poorly understood. Changes in body-representations by the manipulation of natural sounds produced when one’s body impacts on surfaces have recently been evidenced. But will these changes also occur with non-naturalistic sounds, which provide no information about the impact produced by or on the body? Drawing on the well-documented capacity of dynamic changes in pitch to elicit impressions of motion along the vertical plane and of changes in object size, we asked participants to pull on their right index fingertip with their left hand while they were presented with brief sounds of rising, falling or constant pitches, and in the absence of visual information of their hands. Results show an “auditory Pinocchio” effect, with participants feeling and estimating their finger to be longer after the rising pitch condition. These results provide the first evidence that sounds that are not indicative of veridical movement, such as non-naturalistic sounds, can induce a Pinocchio-like change in body-representation when arbitrarily paired with a bodily action.en_UK
dc.language.isoenen_UK
dc.publisherSpringer Natureen_UK
dc.relationTajadura-Jiménez A, Vakali M, Fairhurst MT, Mandrigin A, Bianchi-Berthouze N & Deroy O (2017) Contingent sounds change the mental representation of one’s finger length. Scientific Reports, 7 (1), Art. No.: 5748. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-05870-4en_UK
dc.rightsThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.en_UK
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/en_UK
dc.titleContingent sounds change the mental representation of one’s finger lengthen_UK
dc.typeJournal Articleen_UK
dc.identifier.doi10.1038/s41598-017-05870-4en_UK
dc.citation.jtitleScientific Reportsen_UK
dc.citation.issn2045-2322en_UK
dc.citation.volume7en_UK
dc.citation.issue1en_UK
dc.citation.publicationstatusPublisheden_UK
dc.citation.peerreviewedRefereeden_UK
dc.type.statusVoR - Version of Recorden_UK
dc.contributor.funderArts and Humanities Research Councilen_UK
dc.contributor.funderEconomic and Social Research Councilen_UK
dc.citation.date18/07/2017en_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity College Londonen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity College Londonen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of Londonen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of Warwicken_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity College Londonen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of Londonen_UK
dc.identifier.wtid918947en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0002-3635-9897en_UK
dc.date.accepted2018-06-07en_UK
dcterms.dateAccepted2018-06-07en_UK
dc.date.filedepositdate2018-07-19en_UK
dc.subject.tagMind and Knowledgeen_UK
dc.subject.tagPhilosophy of Mind and Actionen_UK
rioxxterms.apcnot requireden_UK
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_UK
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_UK
local.rioxx.authorTajadura-Jiménez, Ana|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorVakali, Maria|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorFairhurst, Merle T|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorMandrigin, Alisa|0000-0002-3635-9897en_UK
local.rioxx.authorBianchi-Berthouze, Nadia|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorDeroy, Ophelia|en_UK
local.rioxx.projectProject ID unknown|Economic and Social Research Council|http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000269en_UK
local.rioxx.projectProject ID unknown|Arts and Humanities Research Council|http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000267en_UK
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate2018-07-19en_UK
local.rioxx.licencehttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/|2018-07-19|en_UK
local.rioxx.filenames41598-017-05870-4.pdfen_UK
local.rioxx.filecount1en_UK
Appears in Collections:Psychology Journal Articles

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
s41598-017-05870-4.pdfFulltext - Published Version1.44 MBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is protected by original copyright



A file in this item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons

Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.