Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/2608
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorBjarnadottir, Bjorg-
dc.date.accessioned2010-12-13T12:10:19Z-
dc.date.available2010-12-13T12:10:19Z-
dc.date.issued1996-
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/2608-
dc.description.abstractResearch on the deciphering of nonsense words within the context of text, a story, or tale was conducted at various schools and day-care centres in the Stirling area of Scotland in 1985-1988. Three experiments were conducted, in which large samples of primary school children aged 4-12 were tested. The experiments resembled Werner and Kaplan's (1950) "Word-Context Task, " in which isolated sentences in a series with one nonsense word in each sentence were presented to school children. The children were asked to answer questions about the meanings of these words. The results were not in line with the rapid word learning that experience suggests happens in young children, it was not until after age 9 that the children started to give approximately correct answers, and prior to age 11 the answers did not meet up with proper adult definitions. It has been pointed out, however (Donaldson, 1978), that because these sentences were not supported by any relation to immediate context and behaviour, and because the children were required to process utterances as pure isolated language - an unnatural situation for language acquisition - the "Word-Context Task" may have given an unrealistic picture of the child's ability to acquire language naturally. In the three word-leaming studies at Stirling University in 1985-1988, in order to account for a more natural presentation, the sentences with the nonsense word were embodied in the context of a story. Children were thought to fare better (than the children in the Wemer & Kaplan study) when listening to such a story, especially if the basic theme was of interest. A methodological tool, refined in the work of Dockrell (1981), in which the full meaning of a term involves having worked out the sense, reference, and denotation of the term. was applied in each of the test batteries that followed the presentation of the story. In these tests, the children were tested on both their comprehension and production of the new term in question. Drawings were used in order to try to tap the children's denotation of the new term, and to facilitate young children's approach to the demands of the study. As regards word meaning in general. Martin Joos (1972) had argued that the common blunder was that an odd word must have an odd sense--the odder, the better. He argued that one should define words in such a fashion as to make them contribute least to the total message derivable from its passage where it is housed, rather than, e. g., defining it according to some presumed etymology of semantic history. He called this concept "a tacit principle", and argued that word learners and word users would sense the intuitive familiarity of the conveyed meaning of words and text. Words are, according to this principle, "mysterious" in their environment, their meanings are not worked out deliberately, intentionally; rather, one should make the mysterious item maximally supportive and supported in its situation, in order that redundancy would result in proper connotation of the distributed meaning. Context and knowledge of contexts reveal meaning; the text is processed holistically, and so are the instantaneous meanings of the words of which it is composed. Thus, Joos maintained that in deciphering an unknown word, the wisest course is to assume the "least meaning" consistent with the context. Tasks such as Werner and Kaplan's "Word-Context Task" (1950), force subjects to infer aspects of meaning that go well beyond this "least" meaning and, as Joos pointed out, this leads notably to errors from which recovery is difficult. In the studies at Stirling University, attempts were made to determine if different types of learning would result in different types of responses. The dichotomy, intentional/incidental or analytic/holistic was worked out into experimental and control conditions, as based on Aveling's pioneering experiment (1911, 1912) into the general and particular aspects of encoded stimuli. Later, Lee Brooks (1978) worked with the dichotomies intentional/incidental in his Lepton experiments and argued that the more complex a behaviour is (speaking or writing, for example), the more likely it is to be learned implicitly. He pointed out, however, that the dichotomies explicit/implicit, analytic/non analytic, and deliberate vs. intuitive processes need to be elaborated and not taken as a strict division. In the three experiments at Stirling, children of primary school age (ages 4 to 12) were presented with a "word-context" task and their understanding of the unknown word was probed under different conditions. In the control condition a control word was probed, but in the experimental condition the child's understanding of the target word was fully tested. All the children listened to a short story displayed by a video or read from a tape in which the unknown word occurred in several different contexts, the unknown word in each story denoted an unfamiliar natural kind. During the story's display, children in the control condition were, at certain intervals, asked questions about the story's theme. Children in the experimental group were, at these same intervals, shown a sample of objects, to one of which the unknown word referred, and they were asked to hand these objects to the experimenter as she requested the objects, or they were asked direct questions about the meaning of the target word and about other words in the story. After hearing the story, all subjects were tested on their comprehension and production of the unknown word, together with other words, and a scoring procedure based on a technique developed by Dockrell (1981) was applied. This procedure necessitated the full meaning of the term covering aspects of the sense, reference., and denotation of the new term (cf. Lyons, 1977a). The results indicate that children younger than those tested in the Wemer and Kaplan's "Word-Context Task" (ages 8.6 to 13.6) could decipher the full meaning of the new term. But individual differences within age groups showed greater differences than existed between age groups. All in all, the results indicate that working out the full meaning of a new term is a lengthy process indeed (Campbell & Dockrell, 1986), even though a sense of the given semantic domain may often be established quite early in the learning process. Performance styles also differ from younger children to older ones. The results indicate that there were significant age differences between the children in the first and second experiments, but that such differences were lacking in the third experiment, and that control subjects in the three studies seldom gave poorer responses than did experimental subjects and often did better. However, the results must be interpreted in the light of learning and recovery from error occurring, within the experimental subjects in the course of deciphering. If the initial scores of the experimental subjects on the target word as obtained during encoding are compared with the first scores obtained from the control subjects after they had heard the whole story, there is a significant difference in scores between the conditions in favour of the control subjects in all age groups. This is consistent with Joos's assumption that an interference concerning the meaning of a word that occurs too early in the learning task and not enough information of contextual cues will lead the children in the experimental groups astray in their guesses when asked too early for answers on the new word's meaning. But implied in Joos's Axiom is the likelihood for recoveries from errors, and the strategies children use in order to work them out need to be explored further. Much individual variation was found among the children's responses in the age groups. These differences were indeed more significant than were the differences between age groups. Overall, the children did very well at these tasks and that result may be related to the population from which they were selected, Stirling University Playgroup, Psychology Department, Dunblane Primary Schools Nursery and Killin playgroup and primary Schools in Central Scotland. In general, the study provided support for Joos's idea (1958, 1967, 1972) that early interference with the meaning of a new word when deciphering from a verbal context results in errors. However, recovery is possible, if not guaranteed. The study also indicated that children much younger than Werner and Kaplan's subjects (1950) could work out the full meaning of the unknown word. Earlier studies on context such as theirs may have been biased in using experimental setups with bits of isolated language and thus not tapping the real capability of the young word-learner who may come to learn words and their meanings through contexts, through the procedures that the word is linked to, through the verbal and/or nonverbal circumstance it is used in, and accompanied by actions, and exercised; what John Dewey had termed "context of use" (1910). Also, it is clear from the study that at a young age children are able to establish a sense of the given semantic domain a new word belongs to and a stable denotata, as their drawings indicate, but in order to decipher the full meaning of a new term is a lengthy process indeed for most children (Carey, 1978a, Campbell & Dockrell, 1986; Miller & Gildea, 1987). In all age groups and in all conditions there were differences between the children's comprehension and production of the new term in favour of comprehension. However, that result may be related to the nature of the experimental setup (the children gained more experience in working with comprehension than with production), and does not necessarily give support to the claim that comprehension precedes production. There were differences in the age groups between comprehension and production: children in all conditions gave more correct answers in the comprehension tasks and is in accord with earlier experimental evidence. There were children in all age groups who clearly had acquired the full meaning of the new word. its sense, reference, and denotation, whereas the average picture of the children's performance on all these three aspects was quite scattered. This is in accord with earlier work of Dockrell (1981), which suggests that to work out the full meaning of a new term is a lengthy process, even though a sense of the given semantic domain may often be established quite early. In a posttest, two to three weeks later, in which all the children were tested again on the unknown word, it was clear that learning had indeed taken place and that in most cases children who had mapped the new word's meaning in the first experimental set-up, now felt surer about the unknown word's meaning. In general, the study provided support for the view that too early interferences and constraints in the learning task lead the young word learner astray, but that children may nevertheless recover from their errors when provided with more inforination and time. The study also demonstrated that the children were motivated to work at the tasks set before them and that their drawings are a valuable experimental and educational tool for tapping young children's representations. The results indicated that the control group subjects in all age groups fared better than did the experimental subjects, and age group differences here were, in most cases, significant. That the control group subjects fared better is consistent with Joos's assumption that too early an interference on a word's meaning will lead the children astray in their process of deciphering. However, bombarding the children in the experimental groups with questions on the meaning of the unknown word while they are in the process of encoding hampers the needed reorganisation of the tacit semantic representation underlying their meaning of words. The different tests that were designed for the three experiments reveal interesting individual variations in the sample and different testing profiles. The tests show much correlation among them, such that tests that were designed in order to test forverbal coding, comprehension, and general knowledge correlate highly and tests that were designed to test for imaginal coding (depicting) show positive correlation to these verbal comprehension tests - high scores on the verbal tests usually relate to good performance in the depicting tests - and tests that were designed for the production of the new word also show positive correlation with tests that only test for comprehension, and, finally, tests of categorisation that were designed to test for linguistic definitions - for conceptual understanding and declarative skill based on the understanding of logical relationships between words in a taxonomy - show some positive correlation to the other tests (such as the ones of production) but less to others. The test results may indicate that the tests of production and the tests of linguistic definitions better reflect semantic content and true linguistic ability than tests of comprehension, general knowledge, and imaginal coding that may be broader and cover more functions and thus reflect general, cognitive knowledge. These results from the different tests confirm the idea that the meaning of a word has many phases, each of which takes time to grow and to be mastered by the child, the results also show that within the same child and between children there are differences as to their fluency in dealing with these different phases of word meaning and mastering them. Thus, the results confirm the basic notion of this thesis, namely, that different phases are involved in both the composition of a word within text, and in the deciphering of that composition from contextual to acontextual meaning.en
dc.language.isoenen
dc.publisherUniversity of Stirlingen
dc.subject.lcshKnowledge representation (Information theory)en
dc.subject.lcshLinguisticsen
dc.titlePhases of knowledge in lexical acquisition : a developmental study into four to twelve year olds decipherment of unfamiliar words from linguistic contexts during continuous assessmenten
dc.typeThesis or Dissertationen
dc.type.qualificationlevelDoctoralen
dc.type.qualificationnameDoctor of Philosophyen
dc.contributor.affiliationSchool of Natural Sciences-
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychology-
Appears in Collections:Psychology eTheses



This item is protected by original copyright



Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.