Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item:
http://hdl.handle.net/1893/10844
Full metadata record
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Roberts, S Craig | en_UK |
dc.contributor.author | Dunbar, Robin I M | en_UK |
dc.date.accessioned | 2013-02-07T23:06:14Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2013-02-07T23:06:14Z | en_UK |
dc.date.issued | 2000-05 | en_UK |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/1893/10844 | - |
dc.description.abstract | In monogamous species, females often choose between males according to the quality of the territories they defend, but the extent to which females themselves contribute to territory defence is frequently underestimated. Here we test for differences in male and female roles during paired scent-marking bouts, a key component of territorial defence, in a monogamous antelope. In two populations (Kenya, Zimbabwe) of klipspringer, Oreotragus oreotragus, both males and females usually scent-marked at the same site, but there were significant differences between sexes in terms of investment within bouts. Females initiated most bouts, thus dictating the marking strategy of the pair. Males initiated relatively few bouts, but deposited more scent marks per bout than females and were usually the last to scent-mark before leaving the site; they marked on the same branches as the female and thus overmarked her scent. Both sexes deposited more marks during paired than solo visits. Immediately preceding and following scent-marking bouts, males approached females and females left males more often than expected. Female scent-marking rates were higher when they were receptive than at other times, and this increase was matched by elevated marking rates of males. Females may increase marking rates when they are receptive in order to test the quality of their mate or to incite male competition. However, these ideas are unlikely to explain female scent-marking behaviour outside the mating season, which appears to be related primarily to territorial defence. We suggest that these differences in investment in scent-marking bouts are consistent with predictions that females may be autonomously territorial and that overmarking of female scent by males is a form of mate-guarding. | en_UK |
dc.language.iso | en | en_UK |
dc.publisher | Springer | en_UK |
dc.relation | Roberts SC & Dunbar RIM (2000) Female territoriality and the function of scent-marking in a monogamous antelope (Oreotragus oreotragus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47 (6), pp. 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050685 | en_UK |
dc.rights | The publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository. Please use the Request a Copy feature at the foot of the Repository record to request a copy directly from the author. You can only request a copy if you wish to use this work for your own research or private study. | en_UK |
dc.rights.uri | http://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserved | en_UK |
dc.subject | olfaction | en_UK |
dc.subject | klipspringer | en_UK |
dc.subject | territoriality | en_UK |
dc.subject | status signal | en_UK |
dc.subject | monogamy | en_UK |
dc.title | Female territoriality and the function of scent-marking in a monogamous antelope (Oreotragus oreotragus) | en_UK |
dc.type | Journal Article | en_UK |
dc.rights.embargodate | 3000-01-01 | en_UK |
dc.rights.embargoreason | [2000_klipspringer.pdf] The publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository therefore there is an embargo on the full text of the work. | en_UK |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1007/s002650050685 | en_UK |
dc.citation.jtitle | Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | en_UK |
dc.citation.issn | 1432-0762 | en_UK |
dc.citation.issn | 0340-5443 | en_UK |
dc.citation.volume | 47 | en_UK |
dc.citation.issue | 6 | en_UK |
dc.citation.spage | 417 | en_UK |
dc.citation.epage | 423 | en_UK |
dc.citation.publicationstatus | Published | en_UK |
dc.citation.peerreviewed | Refereed | en_UK |
dc.type.status | VoR - Version of Record | en_UK |
dc.author.email | craig.roberts@stir.ac.uk | en_UK |
dc.contributor.affiliation | Psychology | en_UK |
dc.contributor.affiliation | University of Liverpool | en_UK |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000087531600007 | en_UK |
dc.identifier.wtid | 748801 | en_UK |
dc.contributor.orcid | 0000-0002-9641-6101 | en_UK |
dcterms.dateAccepted | 2000-05-31 | en_UK |
dc.date.filedepositdate | 2013-02-04 | en_UK |
rioxxterms.type | Journal Article/Review | en_UK |
rioxxterms.version | VoR | en_UK |
local.rioxx.author | Roberts, S Craig|0000-0002-9641-6101 | en_UK |
local.rioxx.author | Dunbar, Robin I M| | en_UK |
local.rioxx.project | Internal Project|University of Stirling|https://isni.org/isni/0000000122484331 | en_UK |
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate | 3000-01-01 | en_UK |
local.rioxx.licence | http://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserved|| | en_UK |
local.rioxx.filename | 2000_klipspringer.pdf | en_UK |
local.rioxx.filecount | 1 | en_UK |
local.rioxx.source | 0340-5443 | en_UK |
Appears in Collections: | Psychology Journal Articles |
Files in This Item:
File | Description | Size | Format | |
---|---|---|---|---|
2000_klipspringer.pdf | Fulltext - Published Version | 64.7 kB | Adobe PDF | Under Embargo until 3000-01-01 Request a copy |
This item is protected by original copyright |
Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.
The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/
If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.