Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/35842
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorSánchez-Amaro, Alejandroen_UK
dc.contributor.authorBall, Roberten_UK
dc.contributor.authorRossano, Federicoen_UK
dc.date.accessioned2024-03-07T01:11:33Z-
dc.date.available2024-03-07T01:11:33Z-
dc.date.issued2021-04-29en_UK
dc.identifier.other9312en_UK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/35842-
dc.description.abstractSocial primates face conflicts of interest with other partners when their individual and collective interests collide. Despite living in small, primarily bonded, groups compared to other social primates, gibbons are not exempt from these conflicts in their everyday lives. In the current task, we asked whether dyads of gibbons would solve a conflict of interest over food rewards. We presented dyads of gibbons with a situation in which they could decide whether to take an active role and pull a handle to release food rewards at a distance or take a passive role and avoid action. In this situation, the passive partner could take an advantageous position to obtain the rewards over the active partner. Gibbons participated in three conditions: a control condition with no food rewards, a test condition with indirect food rewards and a test condition with direct food rewards. In both test conditions, five rewards were released at a distance from the handle. In addition, the active individual could obtain one extra food reward from the handle in the direct food condition. We found that gibbons acted more often in the two conditions involving food rewards, and waited longer in the indirect compared to the direct food condition, thus suggesting that they understood the task contingencies. Surprisingly, we found that in a majority of dyads, individuals in the active role obtained most of the payoff compared to individuals in the passive role in both food conditions. Furthermore, in some occasions individuals in the active role did not approach the location where the food was released. These results suggest that while gibbons may strategize to maximize benefits in a competitive food task, they often allowed their partners to obtain better rewards. Our results highlight the importance of social tolerance and motivation as drivers promoting cooperation in these species.en_UK
dc.language.isoenen_UK
dc.publisherNature Publishing Group UK Londonen_UK
dc.relationSánchez-Amaro A, Ball R & Rossano F (2021) Gibbon strategies in a food competition task. <i>Scientific reports</i>, 11, Art. No.: 9312.en_UK
dc.rightsThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.en_UK
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/en_UK
dc.titleGibbon strategies in a food competition tasken_UK
dc.typeJournal Articleen_UK
dc.identifier.pmid33927301en_UK
dc.citation.jtitleScientific Reportsen_UK
dc.citation.issn2045-2322en_UK
dc.citation.volume11en_UK
dc.citation.publicationstatusPublisheden_UK
dc.citation.peerreviewedRefereeden_UK
dc.type.statusVoR - Version of Recorden_UK
dc.contributor.funderDeutsche Forschungsgemeinschaften_UK
dc.author.emailalejandro.sanchezamaro@stir.ac.uken_UK
dc.citation.date29/04/2021en_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of California, San Diegoen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of California, San Diegoen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of San Diegoen_UK
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000656206800041en_UK
dc.identifier.scopusid2-s2.0-85105029776en_UK
dc.identifier.wtid1979648en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0003-4036-2455en_UK
dc.date.accepted2021-04-14en_UK
dcterms.dateAccepted2021-04-14en_UK
dc.date.filedepositdate2024-02-28en_UK
rioxxterms.apcnot requireden_UK
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_UK
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_UK
local.rioxx.authorSánchez-Amaro, Alejandro|0000-0003-4036-2455en_UK
local.rioxx.authorBall, Robert|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorRossano, Federico|en_UK
local.rioxx.projectProject ID unknown|Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft|en_UK
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate2024-02-28en_UK
local.rioxx.licencehttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/|2024-02-28|en_UK
local.rioxx.filenames41598-021-88804-5.pdfen_UK
local.rioxx.filecount1en_UK
local.rioxx.source2045-2322en_UK
Appears in Collections:Psychology Journal Articles

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
s41598-021-88804-5.pdfFulltext - Published Version1.27 MBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is protected by original copyright



A file in this item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons

Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.