Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/16732
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorRafetseder, Evaen_UK
dc.contributor.authorPerner, Josefen_UK
dc.date.accessioned2013-09-26T23:22:19Z-
dc.date.available2013-09-26T23:22:19Zen_UK
dc.date.issued2010en_UK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/16732-
dc.description.abstractIn most developmental studies the only error children could make on counterfactual tasks was to answer with the current state of affairs. It was concluded that children who did not show this error are able to reason counterfactually. However, children might have avoided this error by using basic conditional reasoning (Rafetseder, Cristi-Vargas, & Perner, 201022. Perner, J. and Rafetseder, E. 2010. "Counterfactual and other forms of conditional reasoning: Children lost in the nearest possible world". In Understanding counterfactuals/Understanding causation, Edited by: Hoerl, C., McCormack, T. and Beck, S. R. New York: Oxford University Press. View all references). Basic conditional reasoning takes background assumptions represented as conditionals about how the world works. If an antecedent of one of these conditionals is provided by the task, then a likely conclusion can be inferred based only on background assumptions. A critical feature of counterfactual reasoning is that the selection of these additional assumptions is constrained by actual events to which the counterfactual is taken to be counterfactual. In contrast, in basic conditional reasoning one enriches the given antecedent with any plausible assumptions, unconstrained by actual events. In our tasks basic conditional reasoning leads to different answers from counterfactual reasoning. For instance, a doctor, sitting in the park with the intention of reading a paper, is called to an emergency at the swimming pool. The question, "If there had been no emergency, where would the doctor be?" should counterfactually be answered "in the park". But by ignoring the doctor's intentions, and just reasoning from premises about the default location of a hospital doctor who has not been called out to an emergency, one might answer: "in the hospital". Only by 6 years of age did children mostly give correct answers.en_UK
dc.language.isoenen_UK
dc.publisherTaylor and Francis (Routledge) for Psychology Pressen_UK
dc.relationRafetseder E & Perner J (2010) Is reasoning from counterfactual antecedents evidence for counterfactual reasoning?. Thinking and Reasoning, 16 (2), pp. 131-155. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2010.488074en_UK
dc.rightsThe publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository. Please use the Request a Copy feature at the foot of the Repository record to request a copy directly from the author. You can only request a copy if you wish to use this work for your own research or private study.en_UK
dc.rights.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserveden_UK
dc.subjectBasic conditional reasoningen_UK
dc.subjectChildrenen_UK
dc.subjectCounterfactual reasoningen_UK
dc.subjectDevelopmental psychologyen_UK
dc.subjectRealist erroren_UK
dc.subjectReasoning from counterfactual antecedentsen_UK
dc.titleIs reasoning from counterfactual antecedents evidence for counterfactual reasoning?en_UK
dc.typeJournal Articleen_UK
dc.rights.embargodate2999-12-18en_UK
dc.rights.embargoreason[Rafetseder2010a.pdf] The publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository therefore there is an embargo on the full text of the work.en_UK
dc.identifier.doi10.1080/13546783.2010.488074en_UK
dc.citation.jtitleThinking and Reasoningen_UK
dc.citation.issn1464-0708en_UK
dc.citation.issn1354-6783en_UK
dc.citation.volume16en_UK
dc.citation.issue2en_UK
dc.citation.spage131en_UK
dc.citation.epage155en_UK
dc.citation.publicationstatusPublisheden_UK
dc.citation.peerreviewedRefereeden_UK
dc.type.statusVoR - Version of Recorden_UK
dc.author.emaileva.rafetseder@stir.ac.uken_UK
dc.citation.date17/05/2010en_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychologyen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity of Salzburgen_UK
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000277758600002en_UK
dc.identifier.scopusid2-s2.0-77952528191en_UK
dc.identifier.wtid676539en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0002-9816-8607en_UK
dcterms.dateAccepted2010-05-17en_UK
dc.date.filedepositdate2013-09-26en_UK
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_UK
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_UK
local.rioxx.authorRafetseder, Eva|0000-0002-9816-8607en_UK
local.rioxx.authorPerner, Josef|en_UK
local.rioxx.projectInternal Project|University of Stirling|https://isni.org/isni/0000000122484331en_UK
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate2999-12-18en_UK
local.rioxx.licencehttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserved||en_UK
local.rioxx.filenameRafetseder2010a.pdfen_UK
local.rioxx.filecount1en_UK
local.rioxx.source1354-6783en_UK
Appears in Collections:Psychology Journal Articles

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
Rafetseder2010a.pdfFulltext - Published Version307.26 kBAdobe PDFUnder Embargo until 2999-12-18    Request a copy


This item is protected by original copyright



Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.