Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/1893/10904
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorRoberts, S Craigen_UK
dc.contributor.authorGosling, L Morrisen_UK
dc.contributor.authorThornton, E Aen_UK
dc.contributor.authorMcClung, Jen_UK
dc.date.accessioned2013-02-12T23:10:57Z-
dc.date.available2013-02-12T23:10:57Zen_UK
dc.date.issued2001-11en_UK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/1893/10904-
dc.description.abstractThe use by predators of scent marks made by potential prey is a largely unexplored potential cost of olfactory signaling. Here we investigate how animals that differ in their investment in scent-marking respond to simulated predation risk, by comparing the willingness to approach and counter-mark the scent marks of a competitor in the presence or absence of predator odor. We aimed to test whether animals that invest heavily in scent-marking, and which may thus be more vulnerable to eavesdropping predators, will either (1) take greater risks to counter-mark the competitor's scent or (2) reduce or abandon scent-marking. Using outbred male laboratory mice, Mus musculus, we show that, in the absence of predators, individuals which initially scentmark at high frequency (high markers) approach the competitor's scent marks more quickly and spend more time in countermarking than those which initially invest relatively little (low markers). In a sib-sib experimental design, simulated presence of predation risk (urine of ferrets, Mustela putorius furo) caused both kinds of individual to approach the competitor's marks more slowly, but high markers approached more quickly than low markers and spent more time in the vicinity of the competitor's marks. Only high markers significantly reduced their overmarking of the competitor's scent. These results suggest (1) that there is a unique danger inherent to scent-marking at high frequencies and (2) that high-marking males were prepared to accept increased costs of intrasexual competition in order to reduce the risk of predation. Further tests using the scent of naked molerats, Heterocephalus glaber, showed that these effects were not elicited simply by an unfamiliar odor. We discuss reasons for the observed difference in response to predation risk between the groups, and the implications of these results for counter-selection on scent-marking strategies.en_UK
dc.language.isoenen_UK
dc.publisherOxford University Pressen_UK
dc.relationRoberts SC, Gosling LM, Thornton EA & McClung J (2001) Scent-marking by male mice under the risk of predation. Behavioral Ecology, 12 (6), pp. 698-705. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/12.6.698en_UK
dc.rightsThe publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository. Please use the Request a Copy feature at the foot of the Repository record to request a copy directly from the author. You can only request a copy if you wish to use this work for your own research or private study.en_UK
dc.rights.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserveden_UK
dc.subjectmouseen_UK
dc.subjectolfactionen_UK
dc.subjectsignal costen_UK
dc.subjectstatus signalsen_UK
dc.titleScent-marking by male mice under the risk of predationen_UK
dc.typeJournal Articleen_UK
dc.rights.embargodate2999-12-31en_UK
dc.rights.embargoreason[2001_behav_ecol.pdf] The publisher does not allow this work to be made publicly available in this Repository therefore there is an embargo on the full text of the work.en_UK
dc.identifier.doi10.1093/beheco/12.6.698en_UK
dc.citation.jtitleBehavioral Ecologyen_UK
dc.citation.issn1465-7279en_UK
dc.citation.issn1045-2249en_UK
dc.citation.volume12en_UK
dc.citation.issue6en_UK
dc.citation.spage698en_UK
dc.citation.epage705en_UK
dc.citation.publicationstatusPublisheden_UK
dc.citation.peerreviewedRefereeden_UK
dc.type.statusVoR - Version of Recorden_UK
dc.author.emailcraig.roberts@stir.ac.uken_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationPsychologyen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationNewcastle Universityen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationZoological Society of Londonen_UK
dc.contributor.affiliationUniversity College Londonen_UK
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000172103900008en_UK
dc.identifier.wtid748689en_UK
dc.contributor.orcid0000-0002-9641-6101en_UK
dcterms.dateAccepted2001-11-30en_UK
dc.date.filedepositdate2013-02-04en_UK
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen_UK
rioxxterms.versionVoRen_UK
local.rioxx.authorRoberts, S Craig|0000-0002-9641-6101en_UK
local.rioxx.authorGosling, L Morris|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorThornton, E A|en_UK
local.rioxx.authorMcClung, J|en_UK
local.rioxx.projectInternal Project|University of Stirling|https://isni.org/isni/0000000122484331en_UK
local.rioxx.freetoreaddate2999-12-31en_UK
local.rioxx.licencehttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/under-embargo-all-rights-reserved||en_UK
local.rioxx.filename2001_behav_ecol.pdfen_UK
local.rioxx.filecount1en_UK
local.rioxx.source1045-2249en_UK
Appears in Collections:Psychology Journal Articles

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
2001_behav_ecol.pdfFulltext - Published Version124 kBAdobe PDFUnder Permanent Embargo    Request a copy


This item is protected by original copyright



Items in the Repository are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.

The metadata of the records in the Repository are available under the CC0 public domain dedication: No Rights Reserved https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

If you believe that any material held in STORRE infringes copyright, please contact library@stir.ac.uk providing details and we will remove the Work from public display in STORRE and investigate your claim.